Ever run an A/B test where the results looked too good to be true? Or maybe they made no sense at all? You might be dealing with order effects - those sneaky biases that creep in when the sequence of your experiment influences the outcome.
Here's the thing: even the most carefully designed experiments can fall victim to these effects. But there's good news. With the right approach (and a bit of statistical know-how), you can control for these biases and get results you can actually trust.
Order effects are like that friend who always insists on ordering appetizers first - they change how everything else tastes. In experiments, they bias your results by influencing how participants respond over time. And they come in all flavors.
Practice effects happen when people get better just because they've done something before. Think about taking the same personality test twice - you'll probably score differently the second time around, not because you've changed, but because you know what's coming. Fatigue effects are the opposite problem. Run participants through too many tasks and they'll start phoning it in. Then there's carryover effects, where one treatment leaves a lingering impact that messes with the next one.
Why should you care? Because these effects can completely invalidate your findings. You think you're measuring the impact of your new feature, but you're actually measuring how tired people are after clicking through five other variations.
So how do you fight back? Counterbalancing is your first line of defense - you present conditions in different orders across participants. As Statsig's data science team points out, this ensures no single condition always goes first or last. For more complex scenarios, you might need Latin square designs, which we'll dig into next.
When those approaches aren't practical (say you're testing 20 different variations), randomizing condition orders can save the day. It's not perfect, but it spreads the order effects evenly across all your treatments. The key is recognizing these effects exist and building your experiment to handle them from day one.
Latin square designs are the Swiss Army knife of experimental control. Picture a Sudoku puzzle, but instead of numbers, you're arranging experimental conditions. Each treatment appears exactly once in every row and column of your grid.
This isn't some newfangled tech industry invention. Farmers in the 1800s used Latin squares to test fertilizers while controlling for soil quality and irrigation patterns. The team at Number Analytics traces this history, showing how the design evolved from agricultural fields to psychology labs and medical trials.
Here's why it works so well: you're controlling for two different sources of variation at once. Let's say you're testing different onboarding flows. You need to control for both the day of the week (some days are just better for user engagement) and the user's experience level. A Latin square lets you do both without needing hundreds of participants.
The magic happens in the setup. You create your grid, assign treatments systematically, then - and this is crucial - randomize everything. As the Statsig article on first-order effects emphasizes, this randomization step prevents any systematic biases from creeping in. Skip it and you might as well not bother with the fancy design.
But Latin squares aren't a silver bullet. They assume your factors don't interact with each other (spoiler: they often do). And you need equal numbers of everything - treatments, time periods, participant groups. Statistics By Jim breaks down these limitations in detail. When things get more complicated, you might need to level up to Graeco-Latin squares or just embrace a full factorial design.
Building a Latin square is surprisingly straightforward. You need three things: your treatments, your row factor, and your column factor. Let's walk through it.
Start with an n × n grid where n equals your number of treatments. If you're testing four different checkout flows, you need a 4×4 grid. The key is ensuring each treatment appears once per row and column. Sounds simple, but the devil's in the details.
Here's your game plan:
Pick your treatments and identify what you're controlling for (time slots, user segments, whatever)
Build your initial square - start systematic, maybe A-B-C-D in the first row, then rotate
Randomize rows, columns, and treatment labels to eliminate any patterns
This design shines in specific scenarios. Agricultural researchers still love it for controlling soil and irrigation variation. Psychologists use it to manage sequence effects when testing multiple interventions. In tech, it's perfect for feature testing when you're worried about time-of-day effects or user fatigue.
I once saw a team use Latin squares to test four different recommendation algorithms. They controlled for both time of day and user activity level. Without the Latin square, they would've concluded their morning algorithm was best. Turns out, it just happened to run when users were most engaged. The Latin square revealed the real winner - an algorithm that performed consistently across all conditions.
What you're really after are those first-order effects - the direct impact of your treatments. By controlling for other factors, you can confidently say "this checkout flow increased conversions by 15%" instead of "well, maybe it was just because we tested it on Black Friday."
Latin squares pack a serious punch for their simplicity. You're essentially getting three experiments for the price of one - testing your main effect while controlling for two other factors. This efficiency makes them perfect when you're working with limited resources or hard-to-recruit participants.
The precision boost is real too. By removing variation from your row and column factors, you're left with a much cleaner signal. It's like noise-canceling headphones for your data. Each treatment gets a fair shot at proving itself without interference from time effects, participant differences, or whatever else you're controlling for.
But let's be honest about the downsides. The biggest assumption - that your factors don't interact - is often wishful thinking. Maybe your checkout flow works great in the morning but bombs in the afternoon. A Latin square won't catch that interaction. It'll just average everything out and leave you scratching your head.
The equal-numbers requirement is another pain point. Need to test 5 treatments but only have 4 time slots? Too bad. Want to control for 3 user segments but have 5 features to test? You're out of luck. Real-world experiments rarely fit into neat squares.
When Latin squares fall short, you've got options:
Graeco-Latin squares let you control for three factors instead of two
Factorial designs capture all those interactions you're worried about
Mixed models handle unequal group sizes and messy real-world data
The trick is knowing when to use each tool. Latin squares are fantastic for initial screening - quickly testing multiple options with decent control. Once you've narrowed down to your top performers, you can invest in more complex designs to tease out the nuances. Think of it as your experimental design starter kit: not perfect for every situation, but surprisingly effective for most.
Order effects might seem like a niche concern, but they're everywhere once you start looking. That "winning" feature that only wins when tested first? That's an order effect. The user feedback that gets progressively grumpier throughout your usability session? Order effect strikes again.
Latin square designs give you a practical way to fight back. They're not perfect - few statistical tools are - but they're accessible enough to implement and powerful enough to matter. Start small: try a Latin square for your next multi-variant test where you're worried about timing or user effects. You might be surprised what patterns emerge when you control for the noise.
Want to dive deeper? Check out the experimental design course from Penn State or grab a copy of Montgomery's Design and Analysis of Experiments. And if you're running online experiments, platforms like Statsig handle a lot of this complexity for you - though understanding the principles still helps you design better tests.
Hope you find this useful!